INVESTIGADORES
STEIN alejandra
congresos y reuniones científicas
Título:
Future Talk and Lexical Input. A Study with Two Social Groups from Argentina
Autor/es:
ROSEMBERG, C. R.; ALAM, F.; STEIN, A.
Reunión:
Congreso; ELA2012 -Early Language Acquisition; 2012
Resumen:
This study aims to analyze the future talk that
4-year-old Argentinean children from two socio-cultural groups (children from marginalized urban
neighborhoods and children from middle income families with university
education[1]) produce and have the
opportunity to hear in natural discourse set in family and community contexts.
The analysis considers the temporal and sequential terms that children use to
textualize future accounts that are inserted in interactional situations as
well as the temporal and sequential terms that constitute the linguistic input
of both groups of children in their homes.
The theoretical perspective assumes that the
child gradually develops knowledge about temporal and sequential expressions as
they are used by others in discourse and activity contexts and as the child
uses them herself (Levy & Nelson, 1994; Nelson, 1996; 2007). The
acquisition of time concepts is knowledge mediated through language and
cultural artifacts (Nelson, 1996). This development can therefore follow
different trajectories and differences between children can be observed. That
is why this study focuses on the future expressions heard and used by children
from two different socio-cultural groups.
The data consists of a) 228 hours of spontaneous situations (-play, hygiene, meal) recorded in
the homes of 19 children (4:5) from low-income populations and 216 hours of
spontaneous situations (play, hygiene, meal) recorded in the homes of 18
children (4:5) from middle-income populations. Each childs home was recorded
for 12 hours; b) 250 episodes of child talk about future
events produced in spontaneous situations by the same children: 104 produced by
the low-income group and 146 by the middle-income. The units of analysis are
interactive episodes that include a child contribution of at least 2 utterances
referring to a future event. Both corpora were transcribed according to the Code for the Human
Analysis of Transcripts and analyzed using the Computerized Language Analysis (MacWhinney & Snow, 1985). The quantity of the temporal and
sequential vocabulary that children from the two social groups were exposed to
as well as the vocabulary they use in their future accounts were measured.
Findings show differences in the temporal
language used in the textualization process of the future accounts by the children from marginalized urban
neighborhoods (MUN) and by the children from middle-income families with
university education (MI).
Children from urban marginalized neighbourhoods include less temporal and
sequential terms per account than the children from middle-income families
(temporal terms: MUN: 0.5 vs. MI: 1.07; sequential terms: MUN: 0.59; MI: 1.02).
The differences between the groups with regards to temporal terms are
statistically significant (Test used: ANOVA; F(1,36) = 7.53, MSE = 0.399, p <
.01). The diversity of temporal and sequential terms used
by middle income children per account is also higher than the diversity
observed in low-income children (temporal terms: MI: 0.69 vs. MUN: 0.32;
sequential terms: MI: 0.38 vs. MUN: 0.37). The
differences between the groups with regards to the diversity of temporal terms
per account are statistically significant (Test used: ANOVA; F(1,36) = 7.95, MSE =
0.159, p < .01). The same pattern was observed regarding the temporal
and the sequential terms that the two groups of children have the opportunity
to hear in natural situations in their homes and communities. While the
middle-income children hear an average of
189 temporal terms and 250 sequential terms over the course of 12 hours, the
children who live in marginalized urban neighbourhoods hear an average of 117
temporal terms and 157 sequential terms. These
differences are statistically significant (Test
used: ANOVA; temporal terms: F(1, 36) = 13.77, MSE = 3531.5, p < .01; sequential terms:
F(1,36) = 7.7, MSE = 10268.4, p < .01). The diversity of temporal and sequential
terms that middle-income children have the opportunity to hear is also higher
than the diversity observed in the input of low-income children (diversity of temporal terms heard by children in
12 hours: MI: 22 vs. MUN: 10; diversity of sequential terms heard by children
in 12 hours: MI: 10 vs. MUN: 5). These differences are statistically
significant (Test used: ANOVA; diversity of
temporal terms: F(1,
36) = 166, MSE = 8.4, p < .01; diversity of sequential terms: F(1,36)
= 0.141, MSE = 1456.4, p < .01). This data provides new empirical evidence on the importance of taking
into account the linguistic signals that children come in to contact with on a
day-to-day basis (the input) to explain differences in lexical acquisition.
References
Levy, E. and Nelson K. (1994). Words in
discourse: A dialectical approach to the acquisition of meaning and use. Journal of Child Language, 21, 367-389.
MacWhinney, B. and Snow, C. E. (1985). The
Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal
of Child Language, 12, 271-295.
Nelson, K. (1996).
Language in cognitive development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds. Experience,
meaning and memory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
[1] Children from marginalized
urban neighborhoods live with their families in extremely poor communities
called villas de emergencia. These villas de emergencia are
characterized by precarious housing, which has mostly been built from wood and
salvaged materials, and insufficient or inexistent infrastructure services.
Although most of the neighborhoods have drinkable water connections, they all
lack sewer and natural gas lines. In many cases they are illegally connected to
the power grid, since the inhabitants do not have the resources to pay for
service. The neighborhood is accessed by narrow dirt- or cement-floored
corridors. Adults from these communities reached on average 7 years of
schooling. For their part, middle income adults have an average of 17 years of
schooling.