IFIBA   22255
INSTITUTO DE FISICA DE BUENOS AIRES
Unidad Ejecutora - UE
congresos y reuniones científicas
Título:
Object relatives are not always more difficult to process, even in Spanish. Evidence from a study of relative clauses comprehension with psychological predicates
Autor/es:
GATTEI, CAROLINA ANDREA; SHALOM, DIEGO EDGAR; MURUJOSA, MARISOL; SEVILLA, YAMILA
Lugar:
Amherst
Reunión:
Conferencia; CUNY Human Sentence Processing Conference; 2020
Institución organizadora:
University of Massachussets
Resumen:
The asymmetry in the processing of subject (S) and object (O) relative clauses (RC) is well documented in literature and seems to be present in a wide range of languages (e.g. Spanish, English, Dutch, German and French) in language acquisition (e.g. Friedmann et al. 2009), in sentence production (e.g. Belletti & Contemori 2010; in Spanish, Muñoz Pérez & Lago 2012); and in sentence comprehension (e.g. Gordon et al. 2004; in Spanish, Sánchez et al. 2017). However, this asymmetry has only been studied in sentences with transitive activity predicates (TAP), while there has been no evidence reported regarding the processing of RCs with psychological predicates. Since Friedmann et al. (2009), it has been argued that the advantage for SRCs can be explained by the Featural Relativized Minimality theory (fRM; Rizzi 2004) as an effect of syntactic intervention. As both the subject NP and the object NP share the [+N] syntactic feature, the subject NP functions as an intervener when the object NP moves to the left periphery, hindering the establishment of the syntactic dependency. However, the syntactic structure of sentences with third class psychological predicates(TCPP) (Belletti &; Rizzi 1988) such as gustar (?to like?) differs from the structure of sentences with TAPs. Pujalte (2015) argues that in Spanish, ?objects? of TCPPs are generated in a higher syntactic position than ?subjects?, i.e. as high applicative phrases (Pylkkänen 2008). Therefore, the effects of syntactic intervention should occur in the opposite condition and the processing of ORCs with these predicates should entail lower cognitive cost than SRCs. Design: we designed a sentence comprehension task in which participants (n=33) were asked to listen to a sentence; then were showed an image and had to decide whether the sentence they heard was true or false regarding the image they saw. The stimuli (n=20) consisted of RCs with TCPPs (1) and with TAPs (2). In Spanish, TCPPs project the theme of the event with nominative case and the experiencer with dative case, so we chose TAPs that also project the agent of the event with nominative case and the patient with dative case. We manipulated the type of RC with each predicate: RCs with nominative case arguments as the antecedent or ?subject? (1.a and 2.a) and RCs with dative arguments as the antecedent or ?object? (1.b and 2.b). The image showed after the sentence was randomly selected to make the sentence either true or false. Fig. 1 and Fig. 2 show sample picture stimuli for sentences in (1). Response accuracy and reaction times (RTs) were measured during the task. Results: on average, participants answered 89% (SE= .8) of the total stimuli correctly; Table 1 shows the mean of correct answers and standard error per condition; Table 2, the mean of RTs and standard error. Linear mixed-effect models were fitted for data analysis. Results show that RCs with TCPPs were harder to comprehend (p=.02) and were processed more slowly (p<.001) than RCs with TAPs. As it wasexpected, in the case of RCs with TAPs, SRCs were easier to comprehend (p=.003) and faster to process than ORCs (p=.004). In the case of RCs with TCPPs, we found that ORCs were easier to comprehend (p=.007) but that there were no significant differences between ORCs and SRCs RTs (p=.82). Discussion: As predicted, SRCs with TCPPs were more difficult to comprehend than ORCs. These results can be explained as an effect of the intervention of the ?object? NP in the movement of the subject NP to the left periphery, following the fMR proposal. They are also consistent with the fact that not all languages exhibit the same pattern of SRC/ORC asymmetry, depending of the structural properties of these languages (Hsiao & Gibson 2003 for Chinese; Ishizuka et al. 2006 for Japanese; Carreiras et al. 2010 for Basque; Kwon et al. (2013) for Korean). Conclusions: Our results agree to some extent with the predictions of fRM regarding the processing of RCs with TCPPs and point to a structure-dependent account of the RCs processing asymmetries. However, further research is needed to explain the difference between the cognitive effort indicated by the RTs and the responseaccuracy in the case of RCs with TCPPs.BELLETTI, A. & CONTEMORI, C., 2010. ?Intervention and Attraction. On the production ofSubject and Object Relatives by Italian (young) children and adults?. In: Costa, J. et al. (Eds.) LanguageAcquisition and Development: Proceedings of Gala 2009. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Press.BELLETTI, A. & RIZZI, L., 1988. ?Psych-Verbs and θ-Theory?. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 6(3): 291-352.CARREIRAS, M. ET AL, 2010. ?Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque?.Cognition, 115: 79-92.FRIEDMANN, N., ET AL., 2009. ?Relativized relatives: types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies?.Lingua, 119: 67-88.GORDON, P. ET AL., 2004. ?Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity?. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 51: 97-114.HSIAO, F., & GIBSON, E.,2003. ?Processing relative clause in Chinese?.Cognition, 90, 3?27.ISHIZUKA, T., NAKATANI, K., & GIBSON, E., 2006. ?Processing Japanese relative clauses in context?. In: Paperpresented at The 19th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing. New York, NY: CUNYGraduate Center.KWON, NAYOUNG ET AL., 2013.?Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence fromERP data.? Language vol. 89, 3: 537-585.MUÑOZ PEREZ, C. & LAGO, M. S., 2012. ?Asimetría en la elicitación de cláusulas relativas: unestudio sobre el español rioplatense?. Revista Virtual de Estudos da Linguagem, 18: 64-83.PUJALTE, M., 2015. ?Hacia un análisis unificado de los verbos psicológicos estativos en español?. In: Marín, R(Ed.), Los predicados psicológicos. Madrid: Visor.PYLKKÄNEN, L., 2008. Introducing Arguments. Cambridge: MIT Press.RIZZI, L., 2004.?Locality and the left periphery?. In: Belletti, A. (Ed.), Structures and Beyond: the Cartography ofSyntactic Structures, Vol. 3: 223-251.Oxford-New York: OUP.SÁNCHEZ, M. E. ET AL, 2017. ?Comprensión de oraciones con cláusulas relativas. Un estudio comparativo entresujetos con y sin alteraciones del lenguaje?. Panamerican Journal of Neuropsychology, vol. 11.